Simple Science

Cutting edge science explained simply

# Biology# Neuroscience

The Complexity of Color Perception in Natural Light

A study reveals how light variations influence our color perception.

― 6 min read


Color Perception in RealColor Perception in RealLifecolors daily.Light variations shape how we see
Table of Contents

Human color perception relies on light bouncing off surfaces and entering our eyes. When light hits the retina, it triggers signals in different types of cone cells. Each cone type responds to specific wavelengths of light. However, one cone alone cannot provide precise information about the color we see because it loses some details in the signals it sends to our brain. This is known as the principle of univariance. This limitation occurs at the very start of how we process visual information and cannot be fixed later on.

In trichromatic vision, humans have three types of cones that work together, allowing us to see a wide range of colors. For example, if we see monochromatic light at a wavelength of 580 nanometers, we perceive it as yellow. Interestingly, we can achieve the same yellow color by mixing two different wavelengths, like 520 nm and 620 nm light. This shows how our perception of color can be influenced by different light sources.

Moreover, two surfaces that have distinct color properties can appear the same under one type of lighting but look different under another. This situation is known as illuminant Metamerism. It has been found that this phenomenon happens quite frequently and can make color vision challenging for people. For those with dichromacy, who have only two types of cones, these challenges can be even more pronounced.

Real-World Color Perception

Most of the previous research on metamerism was done under controlled conditions where light sources were uniform. However, real-world lighting is not so simple. Natural environments have various light sources and angles, which can change how colors look. Recent studies have shown that the direction and type of light can vary greatly in natural settings. When a surface tilts in this environment, it can catch different types of light, leading to different color perceptions.

In this context, researchers used computer graphics to see how often metamerism occurs when surfaces are placed in a three-dimensional setting that reflects real-world lighting effects. They tested how much the ability to distinguish between colors improves when surfaces are tilted to catch different light angles.

For example, in one scenario, two surfaces under a mix of sunlight and skylight appeared the same at one angle. However, when tilted to sample skylight, the two surfaces became distinguishable.

Research found that trichromats (people with normal color vision) could identify 88.5% of metameric pairs in outdoor settings and 81.5% indoors just by tilting the surface. Interestingly, those with dichromacy (like deutan, protan, and tritan types) showed even higher identification rates in both outdoor and indoor environments.

Color Discriminability

The researchers noted significant differences in how well people with different types of color vision could identify colors based on the type of setting (outdoor or indoor) and their specific color vision type. While the overall ability to distinguish colors improved with tilting, this ability varied across different groups.

To assess color differences, the study used a criteria based on a metric called ΔE in CIECAM02-UCS. This metric helps to quantify color differences in a way that matches human perception better. They checked how well findings held up with different criteria for what counts as a distinguishable color difference.

As they expanded the criteria for color distinction, they found that while the percentages of identified metameric pairs decreased, the conclusion that a large portion could still be solved remained valid. Thus, tilting surfaces in varied lighting conditions increases our ability to differentiate colors.

For both trichromats and dichromats, the increase in color differentiation was significant. This means that in real environments, conditions allow for a better understanding of color than previously thought.

Understanding Color Changes in Natural Light

The difference in color perception based on tilting also raises questions about how we perceive colors. For example, an increase in color difference from a low value to a high value can feel more significant than an increase from a high value to a slightly higher one, even if the absolute change is the same.

The researchers measured not just the absolute differences but also how much these differences represented a proportionate change. They looked at how much better participants were at seeing color differences in outdoor compared to indoor settings. The results showed that the improvements in color perception were generally more remarkable for those with dichromacy.

The Impact of Directional Light Variation

The findings highlight that illuminant metamerism, which arises from the way our cones respond to light, is not as limiting as once believed. In natural environments, the directional light variation can help many color pairs that would otherwise seem the same become distinguishable.

The research emphasizes that while trichromacy offers a basic understanding of color vision, it does not tell the whole story. In everyday life, the signals from our cones are influenced by changing light conditions. Even if someone lacks one type of cone, they can gain color information at different times and angles.

The precise measurement of light variations in natural settings was essential for this research. Researchers used computer graphics to simulate how different surfaces interact with light in three-dimensional environments. They tested 20,132 different surface Reflectances and rendered them under various lighting scenarios and angles.

Collecting Reflectance Data

To conduct this research, the team collected 54,282 reflectance measurements from various items, including flowers, fruits, human skin samples, leaves, and man-made objects. However, they had to refine this data because many surface pairs were too similar, leading to the risk of miscalculating the number of indistinguishable pairs.

After filtering the data based on similarity, they ended up with 20,132 unique reflectance samples for their analysis. These provided a solid base for evaluating how color perception works under natural lighting.

Conclusion

This detailed study reveals significant insights into how color perception operates in real-world settings. The evidence suggests that while our biological systems limit our ability to perceive color, the complex nature of natural lighting offers a way to navigate these challenges. By understanding how different angles and types of light affect color, we can appreciate the richness and complexity of the visual world around us.

Original Source

Title: How many colours can you see? Real environmental lighting increases discriminability of surface colours

Abstract: Color supports object identification. However, two objects that differ in color under one light can appear indiscriminable under a second light. This phenomenon, known as illuminant metamerism, underlies the difficulty faced by consumers of selecting matching fabric or paint colors in a store only to find that they appear not to match under home lighting. The frequency of illuminant metamerism has been evaluated only under single, uniform illuminants. However, in real world conditions, the spectral content of light falling on an object varies with direction (Morimoto et al. 2019), meaning that a surface will sample different spectra depending on its angle within the environment. Here we used computer-graphics techniques to simulate a pair of planar surfaces placed under newly measured hyperspectral illumination maps that quantify the directional variability of real-world lighting environments. We counted the instances of illuminant metamerism that can be solved simply by viewing surfaces tilted to a different direction. Results show that most instances of illuminant metamerism can in theory be resolved for both trichromatic and dichromatic observers. Color deficient observers benefit more than trichromats implying that the directional variability allows the recovery of the missing dimension in their colour vision systems. This study adds a new perspective to the classic trichromatic theory of human vision and emphasizes the importance of carefully considering the environments in which biological vision operates in daily life. It is striking that the physical directional variability available in natural lighting environments substantially mitigates the biological limitations of trichromacy or dichromacy.

Authors: Takuma Morimoto, J. M. M. Linhares, S. M. C. Nascimento, H. E. Smithson

Last Update: 2024-04-23 00:00:00

Language: English

Source URL: https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2024.04.23.590719

Source PDF: https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2024.04.23.590719.full.pdf

Licence: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/

Changes: This summary was created with assistance from AI and may have inaccuracies. For accurate information, please refer to the original source documents linked here.

Thank you to biorxiv for use of its open access interoperability.

More from authors

Similar Articles