Simple Science

Cutting edge science explained simply

# Biology# Animal Behavior and Cognition

New Insights into Fear Learning in Animals

Research reveals how gradual extinction impacts fear responses in rats.

― 8 min read


Fear Learning UnpackedFear Learning Unpackedeffective in managing fear responses.Gradual extinction proves more
Table of Contents

Learning is a vital part of life for both animals and humans. It helps us anticipate what might happen in the future by using information from our past experiences. This ability is crucial for survival. For instance, if an animal hears a sound that it has learned signifies danger, it will become alert or try to escape. Similarly, if an animal detects a smell associated with food, it will approach to eat.

However, what happens when things change? Sometimes, the signals that once indicated danger or food may no longer be reliable. A sound might not always mean that a predator is nearby, and a familiar smell might not always lead to a meal. In these cases, how do animals organize this new information with what they already know?

One way is by updating their existing knowledge, making the old information less trustworthy. Another possibility is that they keep the new information separate from the old, recognizing that certain sounds or smells can mean different things depending on the situation.

Researchers are still trying to figure out what determines which approach animals take when learning. Many current theories suggest that the difference between what an animal expects to happen and what actually occurs (known as prediction error) plays a crucial role in how they store new information in their memory.

The Role of Prediction Error in Learning

According to some models of learning, animals and humans categorize information about experiences and put it into a specific ‘state’ in their memory. This categorization is influenced by Prediction Errors. Simply put, when there is a big difference between what an animal anticipates will happen and what actually happens, it leads to a greater change in their memory.

When a new experience is much like an old one, and there is little discrepancy, the new experience can blend into the previous memory. However, when a new experience is quite different, it is stored separately, keeping the old memory intact.

A common example of this concept can be seen in the study of fear in rats. When rats learn that a tone signals a mild shock, they develop a fear response. If they later hear the tone without the shock multiple times, their fear response starts to diminish; this process is called extinction. Importantly, even after extinction, if they later hear the tone after a break, they might still show fear. This shows that the original fear memory remains even after learning to associate the tone with no shock.

Experiments on Fear Extinction

Fear extinction in rats has been a key area of study for understanding how new experiences interact with existing Memories. In a typical experiment, rats first learn to associate a tone with an unpleasant shock. After this, they hear the tone without the shock several times, which reduces their fearful response.

Researchers have found that this reduction in fear does not mean the original memory is erased. Instead, the original association can still come back under certain conditions, like when time has passed or when the rats are re-exposed to the shock after extinction.

One theory suggests that when the shock is removed suddenly, it creates a large prediction error. This sudden change makes the rats believe that a new memory state is active, separating their fear response from the original experience with the tone and shock.

The Effects of Gradual vs. Standard Extinction

Some research has indicated that a gradual approach to extinction might be more effective than a standard approach where the shock is removed abruptly. In gradual extinction, the intensity of the shock is slowly reduced over time before the tone is presented alone. For example, if a rat is shocked at 0.8 mA initially, it might be reduced to 0.4 mA, then 0.2 mA, and finally to 0 mA.

This gradual reduction leads to smaller prediction errors. Because the changes are less dramatic, the rat integrates the new experiences into the same memory state as the original Conditioning. This might prevent the fear response from returning later when the tone is presented again.

Experiment 1: Comparing Extinction Methods

In the first experiment, researchers set out to see if gradual extinction would lead to less spontaneous recovery of the fear response compared to standard extinction. After conditioning the rats to associate the tone with a strong shock, they split the rats into two groups. One group received standard extinction with the tone presented alone, while the other group gradually reduced the shock intensity over time.

After testing, the researchers found that the rats that underwent gradual extinction showed less fear when the tone was presented again after a break. This suggested that the gradual extinction method helped create a stronger memory that inhibited the return of the fear response.

Experiment 2: Reinstatement After Extinction

The next experiment aimed to determine if the gradual method was also more effective at preventing reinstatement of fear after the rats were exposed to the shock again. The same conditioning procedures were followed, with one group experiencing gradual extinction and another going through standard extinction.

After a two-week break, the rats were tested. The results confirmed that those who received the gradual extinction not only had less spontaneous recovery of fear but also showed less reinstatement after being re-exposed to the shock. This reinforced the idea that a gradual approach leads to a more lasting extinction of fear.

Experiment 3: Checking Shock Intensity Reduction

The researchers wondered if the effectiveness of gradual extinction depended on the order of shock intensity reduction. Therefore, in their next experiment, they created a group that received the same number of weaker shocks but in a random order.

The gradual extinction group still showed less reinstatement of fear compared to the other group that had shock intensity presented randomly. This showed that simply receiving lower shocks was not enough; the key was the progressive reduction in intensity to prevent reinstatement of fear.

Experiment 4: Shock Devaluation vs. Gradual Extinction

In this experiment, the researchers wanted to determine if the effects of gradual extinction were due to a decrease in the shock's intensity instead of the learning process. They compared a group that experienced the gradual reduction of shock intensity with a group that was conditioned with the lowest shock intensity from the start.

The results indicated that the gradual extinction approach was more effective than just starting with a low-intensity shock. This suggested that the gradual method itself was critical for creating a lasting memory of extinction.

Experiment 5: Context Shifts and Extinction Effectiveness

Next, the researchers investigated whether changing the context, such as the physical or temporal environment, affected the gradual extinction process. They trained rats in one context and then attempted extinction in either the same or a different context.

They found that when extinction occurred in a different context, the advantages of gradual extinction were diminished, leading to more spontaneous recovery of fear. This indicated that changes in context could influence how memories are organized and retrieved.

Experiment 6: Delaying Extinction After Conditioning

In this experiment, the researchers examined the effects of delaying extinction after conditioning. The aim was to see if this delay made a difference in the effectiveness of gradual extinction.

The findings revealed that a delay diminished the effectiveness of gradual extinction compared to when it was conducted shortly after conditioning. This demonstrated that timing and context play important roles in how fear memories are formed and managed.

Summary of Findings

Across multiple experiments, the research consistently showed that a gradual extinction protocol was more effective than a standard protocol. By progressively reducing the shock intensity instead of removing it suddenly, the rats displayed less spontaneous recovery and reinstatement of fear.

The key factors contributing to this effect included the reduction in shock intensity being gradual and the timing of the extinction process relative to the conditioning. Furthermore, the results underscored the importance of the context in which learning occurs, indicating that changes in environment could influence how memories are organized and recalled.

Implications for Understanding Memory and Learning

Understanding how animals learn and how memories are formed has significant implications for improving learning strategies and therapies for various behavioral issues. The findings from these experiments suggest that gradual exposure to fear-inducing stimuli, rather than abrupt removal, can lead to more effective long-term management of fear responses.

This research opens new avenues for how we might approach treating phobias and anxiety-related disorders in both animals and humans. It emphasizes the importance of careful timing and context in learning and memory processes.

In conclusion, the research adds to our understanding of the intricate mechanisms of learning and memory in animals, providing valuable insights that can be applied in practical therapeutic settings.

Original Source

Title: Prediction error determines how memories are organized in the brain: a study of Pavlovian fear extinction in rats

Abstract: How is new information organized in memory? According to latent state theories, this is determined by the level of surprise, or prediction error, generated by the new information: small prediction error leads to updating of existing memory, large prediction error leads to encoding of a new memory. We tested this idea using a protocol in which rats were first conditioned to fear a stimulus paired with shock. The stimulus was then gradually extinguished by progressively reducing the shock intensity until the stimulus was presented alone. Consistent with latent state theories, this gradual extinction protocol (small prediction errors) was better than standard extinction (large prediction errors) in producing long-term suppression of fear responses; and the benefit of gradual extinction was due to updating of the conditioning memory with information about extinction. Thus, prediction error determines how new information is organized in memory, and latent state theories adequately describe the ways in which this occurs.

Authors: Nathan M. Holmes, N. G. W. Kennedy, J. C. Lee, S. Killcross, R. F. Westbrook

Last Update: 2024-04-23 00:00:00

Language: English

Source URL: https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2024.01.16.575867

Source PDF: https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2024.01.16.575867.full.pdf

Licence: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/

Changes: This summary was created with assistance from AI and may have inaccuracies. For accurate information, please refer to the original source documents linked here.

Thank you to biorxiv for use of its open access interoperability.

More from authors

Similar Articles