Simple Science

Cutting edge science explained simply

# Computer Science# Computation and Language

Language Development in Neural Networks

Study reveals neural networks can create languages using anaphoric structures.

― 6 min read


Neural Networks andNeural Networks andLanguage Creationanaphoric languages.Neural networks develop their own
Table of Contents

When we talk, we often leave out parts of our sentences that can be understood from the context. For example, instead of saying "Mary is walking and Mary is smiling," we can say "Mary is walking and she is smiling." The word "she" refers back to "Mary," and this is called Anaphora. Anaphora helps us communicate more efficiently by reducing the length of our statements without losing meaning. This style of speaking can confuse the listener because they have to figure out who or what is being referred to by the pronoun or omitted information. Despite the chance for misunderstanding, anaphora is common in many Languages.

This study looks at whether similar anaphoric structures can form in artificial neural networks that are programmed to communicate. We want to find out if these networks can naturally create the same kinds of shortcut language we use. The findings show that neural networks can learn languages that use anaphora without needing a lot of extra rules or guidelines. Interestingly, when we encourage networks to be more Efficient speakers, they use more anaphoric structures.

The Role of Pragmatics in Language

Pragmatics is an important part of communication. It allows us to use language efficiently, utilizing structures like ellipsis and anaphora to shorten our sentences while keeping the original meaning. When a speaker uses an anaphoric structure, they rely on the listener to interpret who or what the pronouns refer to. This might introduce some level of confusion, but anaphora is widely used in human languages because it makes communication quicker and more effective.

For instance, in a conversation, referring back to someone by using a pronoun gives the listener a chance to recall what was just said without repeating the entire name. Similarly, ellipsis allows a speaker to leave out words or phrases, assuming that the listener can infer the missing parts based on the context.

Efficient Communication and Language Structures

Research has shown that languages have evolved to facilitate efficient communication between people. A well-known rule, Zipf's Law, suggests that more common words tend to be shorter than less common ones. This helps speakers express ideas using fewer and simpler words, making it easier and less exhausting to communicate. However, simplifying language can lead to confusion; for example, a listener might struggle to grasp the intended meaning when the speaker leaves out relevant information.

Interestingly, despite this potential for misunderstanding, anaphoric structures still exist in many languages. This suggests that the advantages they bring to speakers-such as saving time and effort-outweigh the risks of miscommunication.

The Signaling Game

To examine how language emerges in neural networks, we design an experiment based on a type of game where one "speaker" agent creates a signal based on a meaning, and another "listener" agent tries to guess the original meaning based on that signal. This setup allows us to observe how agents develop a language without anyone teaching them the rules.

During the experiment, the speaker receives a meaning and sends a signal to the listener. The listener then tries to predict the intended meaning based on that signal. If they guess correctly, the round is a success. The aim is to see if the agents can create a language that includes anaphoric structures on their own.

Learning Anaphoric Structures

To check if neural networks can learn languages with anaphoric structures, we first train a single listener agent using languages that are specially designed to simulate different types of anaphoric structures. In our tests, we created three types of languages: one that includes no omissions, one that uses pronouns for Redundancy, and one that uses pro-drop, where redundant information is simply left out of the signal.

We found that all three types of languages were learnable by the listener agent. However, the speed at which they learned varied among them, with no omission being the easiest to learn, followed by the pronoun language, and lastly, the pro-drop language.

Emergence of Anaphoric Structure

Next, we moved to a multi-agent setup to see if anaphoric structures could appear naturally between agents. We set up two conditions for this experiment: one where we encouraged agents to keep their Signals shorter and another where there were no such efficiency demands.

In every run of the experiment, we noticed that the agents were able to create some form of anaphoric structure, even when they weren't being explicitly pushed to be more efficient. However, when we did apply the pressure for brevity, the use of these structures increased significantly.

Measuring Anaphoric Structure

To evaluate if anaphoric structures were indeed present in the emergent languages, we used three main measurements.

  1. Signal Uniqueness: This measures how distinct the words used to denote redundancy in meaning are. A higher score indicates that the agents have found unique ways to express redundant meanings, which reflects anaphoric structure.

  2. Signal Length: This looks at the average length of the signals sent for different types of meanings. If anaphoric structures were effectively in use, we would expect that redundant meanings would lead to shorter signals.

  3. Predictive Ambiguity: This assesses how uncertain the listener is about the meaning behind the signals they receive. A higher level of ambiguity in redundant meanings compared to non-redundant ones suggests the presence of anaphoric structures.

Findings from the Multi-Agent Experiments

Across all conditions of our experiments, the results indicate that anaphoric structures are indeed present in the languages created by the agents. The emergent languages displayed unique elements that were used only to express redundancy and demonstrated increased ambiguity regarding the intended meanings when redundancy was present.

When a speaker's efficiency was encouraged, these indicators of anaphoric structure became even more pronounced. However, even without such encouragement, these structures appeared naturally during communication. While the overall signal lengths did not dramatically reduce, they reflected the presence of anaphoric structures.

We observed that while languages with anaphoric structures could emerge without explicit efficiency pressures, such pressures played a role in shaping the types of structures that formed.

Conclusion

Our experiments indicate that artificial neural networks can learn and develop languages that use anaphoric structures similarly to human languages. The findings highlight the robustness of anaphoric structures and their ability to emerge from the pressures of communication. Importantly, this shows that such structures do not require strict rules to develop but can arise naturally based on the context of communication.

As these systems evolve, they reveal insights into how language might emerge in human contexts, suggesting fundamental ties between semantics and communication needs. Further studies could explore how to encourage even more complex language structures in artificial agents, shedding light on the intricate relationships within language development.

Original Source

Title: Anaphoric Structure Emerges Between Neural Networks

Abstract: Pragmatics is core to natural language, enabling speakers to communicate efficiently with structures like ellipsis and anaphora that can shorten utterances without loss of meaning. These structures require a listener to interpret an ambiguous form - like a pronoun - and infer the speaker's intended meaning - who that pronoun refers to. Despite potential to introduce ambiguity, anaphora is ubiquitous across human language. In an effort to better understand the origins of anaphoric structure in natural language, we look to see if analogous structures can emerge between artificial neural networks trained to solve a communicative task. We show that: first, despite the potential for increased ambiguity, languages with anaphoric structures are learnable by neural models. Second, anaphoric structures emerge between models 'naturally' without need for additional constraints. Finally, introducing an explicit efficiency pressure on the speaker increases the prevalence of these structures. We conclude that certain pragmatic structures straightforwardly emerge between neural networks, without explicit efficiency pressures, but that the competing needs of speakers and listeners conditions the degree and nature of their emergence.

Authors: Nicholas Edwards, Hannah Rohde, Henry Conklin

Last Update: 2023-08-15 00:00:00

Language: English

Source URL: https://arxiv.org/abs/2308.07984

Source PDF: https://arxiv.org/pdf/2308.07984

Licence: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/

Changes: This summary was created with assistance from AI and may have inaccuracies. For accurate information, please refer to the original source documents linked here.

Thank you to arxiv for use of its open access interoperability.

More from authors

Similar Articles