Disappointment: The Hidden Lessons of Our Brains
Discover how animals and humans cope with disappointment through brain mechanisms.
Hyunchan Lee, Okihide Hikosaka
― 7 min read
Table of Contents
- The Role of Rewards
- The Importance of Expectation
- The Brain Regions Involved
- Lateral Habenula (LHb)
- Periaqueductal Gray (PAG)
- How Do Animals Deal With Disappointment?
- The Experiment Setup
- What Happened During the Task?
- Gaze Reactions: An Indicator of Motivation
- The Brain's Response to Rewards
- The Tonic vs. Phasic Response
- Learning from Disappointment
- Implications for Mental Health
- The Takeaway
- Original Source
- Reference Links
Disappointment is a feeling we all know too well. Whether it's that moment when your favorite snack is not on sale or your favorite show gets canceled after one season, disappointment can be quite a downer. But why does this happen, and how do we, as humans and animals, deal with it? Let’s take a look into the brains of animals to understand what happens when they face disappointment during their quest for rewards.
The Role of Rewards
Rewards are essential for both animals and humans. They motivate us to engage in behaviors that lead to positive outcomes. In the wild, animals hunt for food, and if they succeed, they get to eat! However, sometimes they may face disappointment when their efforts do not yield the expected results.
For instance, imagine a hungry bear trying to catch fish. If it fails to snag a fish after several attempts, the bear might rethink its strategy. This is where the concept of disappointment plays a crucial role in shaping behaviors. Animals adapt their behaviors to conserve energy. If the rewards are scarce, some animals will hibernate or migrate to find better food sources.
The Importance of Expectation
Expectation is a powerful motivator. When we expect a reward, we are more likely to stay engaged in a task. Conversely, when we face a disappointing outcome, our Motivation can plummet. But what signals our brains to keep trying, even after things don’t go as planned?
Scientists believe that certain brain mechanisms help indicate whether we should push through disappointment or simply give up. They looked into specific brain regions to see how disappointment is signaled and how expectations of rewards are maintained.
The Brain Regions Involved
Lateral Habenula (LHb)
One critical player in processing disappointment is the lateral habenula (LHb). Think of it as a mood regulator in the brain. When an animal experiences disappointment, the LHb lights up, sending signals that can lead to a decrease in motivation.
For example, if our bear failed to catch fish, the LHb would signal this disappointment, potentially discouraging it from continuing to fish. This brain region is a key input to dopamine neurons, which are involved in feelings of pleasure and reward. So, when LHb activity increases due to disappointment, dopamine levels can drop, affecting motivation.
Periaqueductal Gray (PAG)
Another important region is the periaqueductal gray (PAG). This area has a different job. Instead of signaling disappointment, the PAG helps maintain a sense of reward expectancy. It tells the brain, “Hey, keep this up; you might still get something good out of it!”
While the LHb responds quickly to disappointment, the PAG provides longer-lasting signals that keep motivation alive, even in the face of setbacks. Think of it as the cheerleader in our brain saying, “Don’t worry; keep going! You’ve got this!”
How Do Animals Deal With Disappointment?
To understand how animals cope with disappointment, researchers ran some interesting experiments, particularly focusing on monkeys. These primates can predict rewards based on their experiences. In a series of tasks, researchers presented monkeys with images representing different rewards and punishments, allowing them to see how their brains reacted to disappointment.
The Experiment Setup
Monkeys were shown different scenes that indicated potential rewards. Some scenes were linked to high-value rewards (like a big helping of juice), while others hinted at low-value rewards (not so great, just a sip of juice). The researchers recorded the monkeys’ gaze, the intensity of their focus on scenes, and the neuronal activity in their brains while they navigated through tasks.
What Happened During the Task?
During these tasks, the monkeys exhibited different levels of visual attention towards the scenes based on the expected rewards. When high-value scenes appeared, the monkeys focused intently, like a child staring at a cookie jar. However, when low-value scenes showed up, their gaze became less focused—like someone staring at a salad while dreaming of cake.
Even when disappointment occurred, such as receiving a low reward, the monkeys still maintained a sense of motivation. This showed that the PAG's activity helped keep their hopes alive. Despite facing a low-value situation, they were ready to tackle the next steps.
Gaze Reactions: An Indicator of Motivation
The monkeys' gaze behavior was very telling. When they encountered disappointing situations, their gaze tended to shift but would return quickly to focus on the task. This suggests a strong commitment to keeping the 'goal' in sight. They showed a remarkable ability to redirect their attention even after disappointment, indicating that they were still in the game.
The Brain's Response to Rewards
Throughout the tasks, both the LHb and PAG were actively recording the monkeys’ responses. As expected, when high-value rewards were available, the LHb’s activity decreased, allowing dopamine levels to rise. In contrast, when disappointment occurred, the LHb signaled the drop in motivational effort by increasing its activity, while the PAG aimed to maintain a sense of hope.
The Tonic vs. Phasic Response
A key takeaway from these studies is the difference between tonic and phasic responses. The LHb primarily responds in a phasic manner, meaning it quickly reacts to feelings of disappointment, kind of like a light switch. The PAG, however, is more like a dimmer switch that gradually adapts to the duration of motivation.
In essence, when the situation looks bleak, the LHb flicks on to warn of disappointment. On the other hand, the PAG keeps things steady, refusing to let the light go out completely. This interplay allows animals (and us) to quickly deal with setbacks without throwing in the towel entirely.
Learning from Disappointment
Interestingly, the ability to learn from disappointment is crucial for both animals and humans. By experiencing failure, animals can adjust their strategies. For instance, if the bear got tired of fishing in one spot after repeated failures, it might venture to a different watering hole instead.
This kind of learning involves not just the LHb but also the PAG, which helps maintain a sense of potential reward in new situations, pushing animals to keep taking risks.
Implications for Mental Health
The findings from studying animals can provide insights into human mental health. Researchers have proposed that dysfunction in the LHb could relate to mood disorders like depression. If the brain struggles with signaling disappointment or maintaining motivation, it might lead to greater feelings of hopelessness.
Furthermore, since the PAG is vital in sustaining reward expectancy, a strong PAG response may help combat feelings of despair. Understanding how these brain regions work together could lead to innovative treatments for mental health conditions.
The Takeaway
Disappointment is an unavoidable part of life, whether you’re a bear trying to catch fish or a human facing a letdown. Understanding how our brains handle disappointment can help us learn to cope better. It’s all about achieving balance: the LHb may dampen our spirits after a letdown, but the PAG works tirelessly to keep our hopes alive.
Next time you face disappointment, remember that your brain is juggling signals to keep you motivated. So, whether it's a canceled show or a missed opportunity, don’t lose sight of the next reward on the horizon! Keep going—your inner cheerleader (thanks to the PAG) is right there with you!
Original Source
Title: Periaqueductal gray passes over disappointment and signals continuity of remaining reward expectancy
Abstract: Disappointment is a vital factor in the learning and adjustment of strategies in reward-seeking behaviors. It helps them conserve energy in environments where rewards are scarce, while also increasing their chances of maximizing rewards by prompting them to escape to environments where richer rewards are anticipated (e.g., migration). However, another key factor in obtaining the reward is the ability to monitor the remaining possibilities of obtaining the outcome and to tolerate the disappointment in order to continue with subsequent actions. The periaqueductal gray (PAG) has been reported as one of the key brain regions in regulating negative emotions and escape behaviors in animals. The present study suggests that the PAG could also play a critical role in inhibiting escape behaviors and facilitating ongoing motivated behaviors to overcome disappointing events. We found that PAG activity is tonically suppressed by reward expectancy as animals engage in a task to acquire a reward outcome. This tonic suppression of PAG activity was sustained during a series of sequential task procedures as long as the expectancy of reward outcomes persisted. Notably, the tonic suppression of PAG activity showed a significant correlation with the persistence of animals reward-seeking behavior while overcoming intermittent disappointing events. This finding highlights that the balance between distinct tonic signaling in the PAG, which signals remaining reward expectancy, and phasic signaling in the LHb, which signals disappointment, could play a crucial role in determining whether animals continue or discontinue reward-seeking behaviors when they encounter an unexpected negative event. This mechanism would be essential for animals to efficiently navigate complex environments with various reward volatilities and ultimately contributes to maximizing their reward acquisition.
Authors: Hyunchan Lee, Okihide Hikosaka
Last Update: 2024-12-17 00:00:00
Language: English
Source URL: https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2024.12.17.628983
Source PDF: https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2024.12.17.628983.full.pdf
Licence: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
Changes: This summary was created with assistance from AI and may have inaccuracies. For accurate information, please refer to the original source documents linked here.
Thank you to biorxiv for use of its open access interoperability.